"They Have Eyes but Cannot See..."
“They have eyes but cannot see…” goes that famous saying. Except the issue isn’t with the eyes… If you don’t believe me, take this famous “selective attention test” (it takes less than 90 seconds). Believe it or not, half the people miss it!
And no, there’s no
discernible pattern to who spots v/s misses it, say the folks who framed that
test in The Invisible Gorilla. Not by age, gender, profession, IQ…
anywhere you conduct the test, about half the people miss it. Almost
unbelievably, they say:
“We
truly are arguing that directing our eyes at something does not guarantee we
will consciously see it.”
How can anyone
possibly say, let alone prove, such a ridiculous statement? Aha, another
scientist ran the gorilla experiment using his eye tracker and found that those
who spotted and those missed it, both spent “a full second looking right at
it”! The brain is apparently very good at filtering stuff, resulting in the
illusion of attention (“we experience less of our visual world than we think we
do”).
This has many
practical consequences. A while back, there was an idea that projecting the
flight information onto (part of) the plane’s front windows would ensure the
pilots were able to see both the flight information and, er, anything else in
their flight path simultaneously. Makes sense, right?
You might have
guessed what flight simulator trials showed. Pilots using the new projection
tech missed another “(simulated) plane on the runway” just as often as pilots
who used the old system! Why are we missing those gorillas and planes?
One, if it’s not
something we expect, we don’t register it (plane, gorilla, whatever). Two, it
matters how much of our attention is “free” at that instant. If we’re doing
something taxing (like counting basketball passes, or landing a plane), the
brain has less processing power left for registering other stuff.
The second point
incidentally explains why hands-free headsets while driving don’t help much.
The issue isn’t with our hands, it’s that the total attention we can summon is
limited. A phone call, hands-free or not, uses up part of our attention leaving
less of it (attention) for driving the car. It also explains why doctors who
are scanning a medical image for, say, tumours, can miss other seemingly
obvious stuff (like a guidewire left inside the patient during an earlier
surgery). In the doctors’ case, both points apply: they don’t expect to see
guidewires inside patients, and they’re concentrating hard to spot a tumour.
You might be
wondering:
“If
this illusion of attention is so pervasive, how has our species survived to
write about it? Why weren’t our would-be ancestors all eaten by unnoticed
predators?”
Great questions.
And the answer is that it is only in modern times that we have more and more
attention-demanding situations. Also, the time to react has reduced with our
technological advances:
“When
you are walking, a delay of a few seconds in noticing an unexpected event is
likely inconsequential.”
That’s not true if
you’re driving, where a split-second delay could be fatal.
Worst of all? Since, by definition, we don’t even know of all those instances of attentional blindness (we don’t know what didn’t register), we have very little feedback telling us of the problem! How then can we improve on that front? Sadly, there’s no answer to this. The best we can do for now is to be aware of this limitation.
Comments
Post a Comment