Noise #3: Its Constituents, and Ways to Minimize it
What are the common sources of noise in decision making? Well, people have selective attention, and selective recall of the information, write the authors of Noise. Information is rarely coherent, it is often conflicting, and so people pick and drop parts. Therefore, what goes into the decision varies across individuals.
People are
different. Some are strict, others lenient; some are optimistic, others
pessimistic. You get the idea. If that sounds like bias, you’re right – it is
actually both. It’s noise too because different people have different
biases. This is called “level noise” – different people have
different base levels.
People makes
exceptions to their own rules. They also react differently if the same problem
is presented in a different context. All these variations within the
same person are called “pattern noise”.
Within pattern
noise, there is a subcategory called the “occasion noise” – the mood of
the person can produce different decisions. The “when” matters more than the
“what” at such times.
The authors then
switch to finding steps to reduce noise. We’ll look at a few of them in this
blog.
More heads are
better than one. That’s true, but only under certain conditions: (1) people
should have different specialties and/or preferences, and (2) they should hear
each other’s views only after they have put theirs down on paper first.
But all too often, these conditions are ignored, in which case even if more
people were involved in the decision, they aren’t independent of each other and
thus don’t help reduce the noise:
“Independence
is a prerequisite for the wisdom of the crowds.”
Unintuitively, a
formula that is based on an individual’s decisions in the past, even a crude
one that is “almost a caricature”, still “outpredicted the professional on
which it was based”!
“The
ersatz was better than the original product.”
This is hard to
digest. After all, most of us consider judgment to be “complex, rich, and
interesting precisely because it does not fit simple rules”. The simplified
model, by definition, cannot add anything, it can only “subtract and simplify”.
What does this say about those intangible, instinctive aspects to judgment?
“You
may believe that you are subtler, more insightful, and more nuanced than the
linear caricature of your thinking. But in fact, you are mostly noisier.”
Another
unintuitive thing studies have found is that adding more rules doesn’t improve
the quality of decisions. In fact, more rules usually make things worse. How
can that be? It turns out most rules aren’t independent of each other; they are
correlated with each other. So when one rule is wrong, the lack of independence
means the other rules just compounds the error. But that doesn’t fit with how
AI/machine-learning algorithms work. They use a lot more data points to
decide, not less. What’s going on? They work better because they are able to
notice relationships that humans fail to see.
If all this is
true, why don’t people switch to rule-based decision making, whether by
following it themselves or handing control to AI? Partly, because it is very unsatisfying
to leave everything to cold, impersonal rules. But also, the algorithm isn’t
perfect; it’s just better than us, a distinction we fail to remember:
“(People)
stop trusting it (algorithm) as soon as they see that it makes mistakes.”
Another reason
lies in the scales used by different people. The lack of standardized scales
means everyone assigns different values, often wildly different. A solution
might therefore be to show similar cases as “anchors” – use that for reference,
as a comparative item, and then decide.
None of the above is intuitive or easy, which is why noise is so pervasive.
Comments
Post a Comment